Avoiding Virtual Obstacles During Treadmill Gait in Parkinson’s Disease
Chiahao Lu*, Emily Twedell†, Reem Elbasher, Michael McCabe†, Colum D. MacKinnon and Scott E. Cooper
Department of Neurology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
Falls often occur due to spontaneous loss of balance, but tripping over an obstacle during gait is also a frequent cause of falls (Sheldon, 1960; Stolze et al., 2004). Obstacle avoidance requires that appropriate modifications of the ongoing cyclical movement be initiated and completed in time. We evaluated the available response time to avoid a virtual obstacle in 26 Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients (in the off-medication state) and 26 controls (18 elderly and 8 young), using a virtual obstacle avoidance task during visually cued treadmill walking. To maintain a stable baseline of stride length and visual attention, participants stepped on virtual “stepping stones” projected onto a treadmill belt. Treadmill speed and stepping stone spacing were matched to overground walking (speed and stride length) for each individual. Unpredictably, a stepping stone changed color, indicating that it was an obstacle. Participants were instructed to try to step short to avoid the obstacle. By using an obstacle that appeared at a precise instant, this task probed the time interval required for processing new information and implementing gait cycle modifications. Probability of successful avoidance of an obstacle was strongly associated with the time of obstacle appearance, with earlier-appearing obstacles being more easily avoided. Age was positively correlated (p < 0.001) with the time required to successfully avoid obstacles. Nonetheless, the PD group required significantly more time than controls (p = 0.001) to achieve equivalent obstacle-avoidance success rates after accounting for the effect of age. Slowing of gait adaptability could contribute to high fall risk in elderly and PD. Possible mechanisms may include disturbances in motor planning, movement execution, or disordered response inhibition.IntroductionParkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease that leads to a progressive decline in motor function, including symptoms of rigidity, tremor, bradykinesia, and postural instability and gait disorder. The gait disorder is one of the most disabling motor symptoms of PD, and at the same time, one of the most refractory to treatment, such as pharmacological (e.g., levodopa) and neuromodulatory (e.g., deep brain stimulation) treatments. One debilitating consequence of postural instability and gait disorders is increased fall risk. Over 60 percent of people with PD reported at least one fall and 70 percent of these people experienced 2 falls within a year (Wood et al., 2002). The prevalence of falls also significantly increases with disease progression (Hiorth et al., 2014).Falling can often occur when individuals with PD fail to negotiate an obstacle during walking (Imms et al., 1977; Campbell et al., 1990; Chen et al., 1996; Defer et al., 1999; Galna et al., 2009; Brown et al., 2010). To prevent this from happening, one needs to inhibit the preplanned step and modify the ongoing cyclical movement to avoid the obstacle, i.e., a form of gait adaptation. Previous research on obstacle avoidance has indicated that movement performance was worse in people with PD compared to healthy individuals. This includes slower crossing speed (Brown et al., 2010), shorter stride length, greater stride and stance phase duration (Stegemöller et al., 2012; Vitório et al., 2010), and a larger step width (Galna et al., 2013b) and an increased number of steps when the obstacle appeared (Caetano et al., 2018). In addition, these gait impairments appear to be associated with the severity of the overall motor symptoms (Michel et al., 2009; Stegemöller et al., 2012; Galna et al., 2013b; Caetano et al., 2018).While the spatial characteristics of movements to avoidance obstacles have been widely studied, it is also important to understand how the temporal presentation of an obstacle affects the performance. Previous literature in healthy adults has indicated that the timing of obstacle presentation can have a substantial impact on success rate for obstacle avoidance in both young and older healthy adults (Chen et al., 1994, 1996). Their results show that the success rate increases with the available response time relative to obstacle appearance. To our knowledge, no study has investigated how timing of obstacle appearance in people with PD affects their performance with respect to the available response time.We examined obstacle avoidance in relation to available response time in people with PD compared to healthy controls during treadmill walking. In order to evaluate the performance of avoiding obstacles independent of difficulty keeping up with the speed of the treadmill, we measured each individual’s preferred overground walking speed, and examined obstacle avoidance at that speed. We hypothesized that: (1) people with PD would require more response time for obstacle negotiation compared with healthy controls, and (2) the required response time would be associated with overall disease severity in people with PD.Materials and MethodsParticipantsFifty-two participants (demographics in Table 1) were included in the study: 26 people with PD and 26 healthy controls. All patients included in the study had idiopathic PD (Hoehn and Yahr scale of II–IV) (Hoehn and Yahr, 1967). Participants with PD were tested in the morning after 12-h withdrawal from anti-parkinson’s medications (practically defined off-medication state) (Defer et al., 1999). Eight of the PD participants had implanted deep brain stimulators. In these individuals, stimulation was turned off at least 1-h before the testing. Exclusion criteria included medical conditions impairing gait except for PD and diagnosis of dementia or a score of 45, X2(1,42) = 0.31, p = 0.58].A small fraction of obstacle events was excluded from the analysis (0.5% in Control and 2.7% in PD) due to: footfalls not aligned with stepping stones on the gait cycle preceding the obstacle, when the participant avoided the obstacle by stepping to one side of it, or by keeping the ankle dorsiflexed so as to bear weight only on the heel. The performance of obstacle avoidance by each group is listed in Table 2. About 4 out of 5 successes were classified as stepping short (as instructed) in both control (mean ± SD, 77.6 ± 16.7%) and PD (80.6 ± 13.2%) groups.TABLE 2Table 2. Summary of performance in obstacle avoidance (mean ± SD).The average ttr50 value was approximately 1.2 times greater in the PD group compared to healthy controls (Figure 2A). A simple between-group comparison was significant [t(50) = -4.4, p < 0.001]. The ANCOVA controlling for age confirmed the results showing a significant main effect of group in the ttr50 [F(1,49) = 12.2, p = 0.001]. The age covariate was also significant [F(1,49) = 13.4, p < 0.001, Figure 3A]. For further confirmation, a simple between-group comparison, excluding participants younger than 45 years, was also significant [t(41) = -3.1, p = 0.003]. Results were the same when we repeated this analysis using raw obstacle latency rather than available time to respond (see Supplementary Figure S2).FIGURE 2Figure 2. (A) The available time to respond (ttr50) of the two groups (the three asterisks shows significant difference between the groups with p < 0.001); (B) The overall success rates for obstacle avoidance task of the two groups.FIGURE 3Figure 3. (A) Relationship between ttr50 and age for both groups, for regression lines, dashed trace represents Parkinson’s disease (PD) and solid trace Control, while light gray circle and dark gray circle represents PD and Control, respectively (age covariate, p < 0.001); (B) Relationship between ttr50 and MDS-UPDRS total motor scores for the PD group (two participant excluded due to missing items in MDS-UPDRS motor, dashed trace represents the regression line, p = 0.16); (C) Relationship between ttr50 and MDS-UPDRS motor axial subscores for the PD group, dashed trace represents the regression line (one participant excluded due to missing axial item, p = 0.02); (D) Relationship between ttr50 and disease duration for the PD group, dashed trace represents the regression line (p = 0.77).In the secondary analysis, success rates were higher in controls than in PD participants (Table 2), but this difference did not reach significance in ANCOVA [F(1,49) = 0.16, p = 0.69; t-test, t(41) = 0.06, p = 0.95, Figure 2B] though the age covariate was significant [F(1,49) = 9.7, p = 0.003]. We also found no significant group effect for short-step success rates [t(41) = -0.78, p = 0.44] in participants older than 45 years of age. For overstep success rate, the between-group difference in participants older than 45 years of age was also not significant (U = 274, p = 0.24).Linear regression analysis in the PD group showed MDS-UPDRS motor score [F(1,22) = 2.08, p = 0.08, R2 = 0.08, two participants excluded due to incomplete scores] was not significantly related to the ttr50 although there was a non-significant trend to increasing ttr50 with increasing motor scores in MDS-UPDRS (Figure 3B). However, the MDS-UPDRS motor axial subscore [Factor 1 items in Goetz et al. (2008)] was significantly related to ttr50 [F(1,23) = 5.14 (missing data in one participant), p = 0.03, R2 = 0.15, Figure 3C]. There was no significant relationship between ttr50 and disease duration [F(1,24) = 0.58, p = 0.45, R2 = 0.02] although there was a non-significant trend to increasing ttr50 with increasing disease duration (Figure 3D). There was no relation of success rate to MDS-UPDRS motor scores [F(1,22) = 0.39, p = 0.54, R2 = 0.03], MDS-UPDRS motor axial subscores [F(1,23) = 2.07, p = 0.16, R2 = 0.04] and disease duration [F(1,24) = 0.07, p = 0.79, R2 = 0.04]. In addition, there was no difference in ttr50 between PD patients with vs. without deep brain stimulator [DBS, t(24) = -1.91, p = 0.07] (All DBS participants tested OFF-stim, with at least 1-h washout) and in ttr50 between more vs. less affected legs in PD patients [paired t-test, t(24) = 0.02, p = 0.98].DiscussionTo our knowledge, this is the first study to assess how PD affects the capacity for obstacle avoidance as a function of the available time to respond. Our results showed that the probability of successfully avoiding an unexpected obstacle depended strongly on the available time to respond in both controls and people with PD. In addition, we found that PD participants required more time than controls to achieve an equivalent probability of avoiding the obstacle.Our control participants replicate the finding by Potocanac et al. (2014), who showed a similar dependence on time-to-respond in a smaller sample of neurologically healthy participants (Potocanac et al., 2014). In addition to being larger, our sample of control participants also spanned a wider age range and included elderly participants. We also varied time-to-respond with higher resolution, i.e., continuously, rather than in discrete increments. The logistic regression curve relating the probability of successful avoidance to available time was shifted to the right in older as compared to younger Controls (i.e., older participants required more time than younger ones to achieve an equivalent probability of avoiding the obstacle).The logistic regression was also shifted to the right in the PD group relative to Controls (i.e., PD participants required more time than Controls to achieve an equivalent probability of avoiding the obstacle) even after accounting for the effect of age. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis, our results demonstrate that people with PD required more time to respond (greater ttr50) in order to avoid obstacles during treadmill walking compared to healthy adults. Although ttr50 increased with increasing disease duration and with increasing MDS-UPDRS total motor score, neither relation reached significance. However, ttr50 was significantly related to MDS-UPDRS axial subscores. This suggests that prolongation of ttr50 is predominantly associated with postural and gait dysfunction, and thus, may be a distinct domain of impairment rather than more generalized measures of disease severity.Previous obstacle avoidance studies in people with PD used a treadmill and three-dimensional obstacles (van Hedel et al., 2006; Michel et al., 2009; Snijders et al., 2010; Nanhoe-Mahabier et al., 2012; Stegemöller et al., 2012). With three-dimensional obstacles the larger limb movement required to step over the obstacle may be additionally difficult for Parkinsonian patients due to hypometria, a spatial deficit, thereby confounding spatial and temporal impairment. In contrast, we used two-dimensional virtual obstacles with instructions to shorten the step to avoid the obstacle. This reduction in the spatial demands of the task allowed a more direct comparison of the temporal aspect of obstacle avoidance between Control and PD. In addition, although we expected the short-step instructions would provoke episodes of freezing of gait (FOG) (Chee et al., 2009), such episodes were actually rare, even in patients for whom FOG was a prominent symptom. This might simply reflect the irregular and unpredictable nature of FOG which makes it hard to provoke reproducibly in a laboratory setting, or the facilitatory effect of the visual cues (“stepping stones”) in our experiments.Shorter steps (in PD) and faster gait (in controls) reduce the available time to respond to an obstacle, for a given obstacle latency, which would be expected to reduce success probability. However, this cannot explain the difference we saw between PD and control subjects, because we analyzed success probability as a function of time-to-respond, rather than of the obstacle latency. Therefore, the prolonged response time in our PD participants cannot be completely explained by walking speed or step length.The obstacle avoidance task used in the current study is similar to a classical response inhibition task (Go-NoGo or Stop Signal Task). People with PD do exhibit impaired response inhibition (Gauggel et al., 2004; Dirnberger and Jahanshahi, 2013). In our task, participants were asked to alter their stride to avoid an obstacle. This differs from a simple response inhibition task as it may require not only inhibiting the default “prepotent” response, but also replacing it with a different one, much like a Stroop test (Stroop, 1935). Furthermore, we observed three different behaviors in response to the obstacle appearance. Not only did we see the prepotent (stepping on the obstacle) and the instructed response (stepping short of the obstacle), but subjects also demonstrated a third behavior, that is, overstepping so that the foot landed on the far side of the obstacle. Overstepping was deployed appropriately to achieve obstacle avoidance, but was contrary to instructions. This appears to represent a preferred obstacle-avoidance response. The fact that overstepping occurred preferentially for obstacles with shorter time to respond (see Supplementary Material) suggests that this strategy is another prepotent response which must be inhibited in order to comply with task instructions. Another potential explanation for overstepping could be that lengthening the step would increase the anterior-posterior base of support, thus providing increased stability in the sagittal plane. Response inhibition is often studied in highly artificial tasks. The present study may shed light on how response inhibition modulates a real-world, and functionally very important daily activity, namely gait.Functional neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that the prefrontal cortex plays an important role in response inhibition or response switching. Maidan et al. (2016) showed a significant increase in prefrontal activation during obstacle negotiation compared to normal walking in people with PD, but this increase was not seen in aged matched healthy adults (Maidan et al., 2016). This is in keeping with the growing body of evidence relating prefrontal cortical attentional and executive function to gait (Herman et al., 2010; Lord et al., 2010; Snijders et al., 2010; Smulders et al., 2013).Gait speed may be a proxy for PD disease stage (Paker et al., 2015), and in our PD participants, walking speed was inversely related to MDS-UPDRS motor scores. A factor that may have contributed to reduced walking speed and shortened step length during treadmill walking in the PD group is visual information load. Participants with PD would perceive more stepping stones given the same projection area on the treadmill belt compared to control participants who walked with greater speed and step length. Thus, this additional load of visual information may exacerbate the performance deficit of these participants, which may, in part, contribute to the prolonged time to respond.Success rates were higher in controls than in PD participants, but this difference did not reach significance. Relatively high success rates in PD patients may be a characteristic of externally cued tasks (van Hedel et al., 2006) or occur when subjects engage a subcortical “fast adjustment network” (Snijders et al., 2010; Potocanac and Duysens, 2017). We also did not find a significant relationship between success rate and the MDS-UPDRS motor scores (PD only). This is expected because success rate depends on obstacle latency. To compare success rates, time-to-respond should be matched, or alternatively, as in the present paper, one can directly compare the function relating success probability to time-to-respond.In the current study, obstacle avoidance occurred in the context of a baseline walking task in which participants were asked to step on virtual stepping stones. The reason for this instruction was to maintain a stable baseline of stride length and visual attention, so that the step preceding an obstacle was similar for every obstacle. However, our use of stepping stones could have affected our results in several ways. First, although we attempted to set the spacing of the stones to match the individual participant’s overground step length, the average of left and right step lengths was used. Since most PD participants had asymmetrical step lengths, this may have normalized the asymmetry of their treadmill gait. Future studies may investigate how gait asymmetry affects obstacle avoidance in PD since it has been shown that elderly with high fall risk may demonstrate more asymmetry in lower limbs when stepping over obstacles compared to young control and elderly with low fall risk (Di Fabio et al., 2004). Second, it is well known that providing a visual cue improves gait in PD (Lim et al., 2005). Third, the requirement to step on the stepping stone may have constituted an additional cognitive load on participants, on which the obstacle-avoidance requirement was superimposed. Therefore, a potential explanation of the increase of the available time to respond in the PD group could be the cognitive cost of dual task, which involved processing of both stepping stones and obstacles. Yet, previous research has shown that simultaneous external cueing did not affect obstacle crossing performance in PD groups with and without FOG during treadmill walking (Nanhoe-Mahabier et al., 2012) (although in that study, the cues were auditory rather than visual).It is worth mentioning that the average available response time of our neurologically healthy older controls in obstacle crossing was higher (0.6 s) than in a previous study (0.2–0.35 s) (Weerdesteyn et al., 2005a) for similar success rates. The difference could be that the average treadmill speed was substantially slower in the Weerdesteyn et al. (2005a) study compared to the current study (0.8 vs. 1.2 m/s). In addition, their obstacle always dropped at the same location and the sound of the obstacle landing may have acted as a supplemental acoustic cue. Chen et al. (1994) reported 50% success at 280 ms available response time in healthy elderly participants when using a virtual obstacle during overground gait. The constraint on gait speed regulation imposed by the use of a treadmill in our study may have increased the difficulty of our task through a dual-task effect and thus increased the response times. Other implementation details (e.g., visual salience of the obstacles) may also have played a role.Our study has limitations, and the findings should be interpreted with caution. First, gait could differ between overground and treadmill walking (Bello et al., 2014; Malatesta et al., 2017), although the literature on this topic is equivocal (e.g., Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2005; Riley et al., 2007; Hollman et al., 2016) and may depend on the specific population (e.g., Watt et al., 2010) and on laboratory-specific details. Second, it is possible that the gait speed and step length which were natural for overground walking were less natural on the treadmill. In that case, our results may pertain more to more challenging gait. The gait speed we chose was intended to approximate “natural,” i.e., overground gait, subject to the constraint that the experiment required a treadmill. Overground gait seemed more relevant to community ambulation, activities of daily living and fall risk, etc., Although we instructed participants to step short of the obstacle, yet overstepping sometimes occurred. Thus, participants had two strategies that could be implemented, but were instructed to preferentially select one of these (step short). The process of selection and suppression of strategies may have increased response time since this could impose an additional cognitive load analogous to dual tasking, which is known to affect gait in PD (Bond and Morris, 2000; Yogev et al., 2005; Rochester et al., 2008; Amboni et al., 2013) (see Supplementary Material for a more detailed analysis of short-stepping/overstepping rates). Future studies are warranted to investigate how the adopted avoidance strategy is influenced by the available time to respond without the constraint of specific instructions. In addition, we were unable to extrapolate our findings to potential fall risk due to lack of comprehensive falls history of our sample population. In order to avoid statistical confounding from systematic differences in step length and gait speed between PD and control groups, we analyzed our data in terms of available time to respond, based on obstacle latency adjusted for gait speed and step length. A limitation of this approach is that that one cannot disentangle effects of speed and target spacing. However, our conclusions were unaffected when data was reanalyzed in terms of raw, unadjusted obstacle latency (see Supplementary Material). Another limitation was that we were unable to assess the effect of learning or fatigue on the obstacle avoidance by comparing earlier vs. later trials, or earlier vs. later obstacles within a trial. This is because we pooled all trials in order to fit the logistic regression with the best accuracy. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the difference between PD participants and controls was that the PD participants required more practice to learn the task. Finally, the obstacle latency was estimated at 60 Hz due to the time resolution of the video.This study shows that the available time required to respond in obstacle negotiation was prolonged in people with PD during preferred speed treadmill gait compared to neurologically healthy adults. Furthermore, the prolonged response time was associated with the severity of Parkinsonian axial motor dysfunction.Author ContributionsCL and SC conceived the study, analyzed the data, developed the methodology, administered the project, and wrote, reviewed, and edited the manuscript. CM conceived the study, investigated the results, developed the methodology, and wrote, reviewed, and edited the manuscript. ET, RE, and MM developed the methodology, analyzed the data, and reviewed and edited the manuscript.FundingThis work was supported by the University of Minnesota Neuromodulation Innovations (MnDrive), the Udall Center grant of the National Institutes of Health Award Number P50NS098573, and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health Award Number UL1TR000114.Conflict of Interest StatementThe authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.Supplementary MaterialThe Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00076/full#supplementary-materialReferencesAmboni, M., Barone, P., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2013). Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: evidence and implications. Mov. Disord. 28, 1520–1533. doi: 10.1002/mds.25674PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarBello, O., Sanchez, J. A., Vazquez-Santos, C., and Fernandez-Del-Olmo, M. (2014). Spatiotemporal parameters of gait during treadmill and overground walking in Parkinson’s disease. J. Parkinsons. Dis. 4, 33–36. doi: 10.3233/JPD-130251PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarBond, J. M., and Morris, M. (2000). Goal-directed secondary motor tasks: their effects on gait in subjects with Parkinson disease. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 81, 110–116. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2000.0810110PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarBrown, L. A., Bruin, N., Doan, J. B., Suchowersky, O., and Hu, B. (2010). Obstacle crossing among people with Parkinson disease is influenced by concurrent music. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 47:225. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2009.10.0171PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarBrown, L. A., Doan, J. B., McKenzie, N. C., and Cooper, S. A. (2006). Anxiety-mediated gait adaptations reduce errors of obstacle negotiation among younger and older adults: implications for fall risk. Gait Posture 24, 418–423. doi: 10.1016/J.GAITPOST.2005.09.013PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarCaetano, M. J. D., Lord, S. R., Allen, N. E., Brodie, M. A., Song, J., Paul, S. S., et al. (2018). Stepping reaction time and gait adaptability are significantly impaired in people with Parkinson’s disease: implications for fall risk. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 47, 32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.11.340PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarCampbell, A. J., Borrie, M. J., Spears, G. F., Jackson, S. L., Brown, J. S., and Fitzgerald, J. L. (1990). Circumstances and consequences of falls experienced by a community population 70 years and over during a prospective study. Age Ageing 19, 136–141. doi: 10.1093/ageing/19.2.136PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarChee, R., Murphy, A., Danoudis, M., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., and Iansek, R. (2009). Gait freezing in Parkinson’s disease and the stride length sequence effect interaction. Brain 132, 2151–2160. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp053PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarChen, H. C., Ashton-Miller, J. A., Alexander, N. B., and Schultz, A. B. (1994). Effects of age and available response time on ability to step over an obstacle. J. Gerontol. 49, M227–M233. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.5.M227CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarChen, H. C., Schultz, A. B., Ashton-Miller, J. A., Giordani, B., Alexander, N. B., and Guire, K. E. (1996). Stepping over obstacles: dividing attention impairs performance of old more than young adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 51, M116–M122. doi: 10.1093/gerona/51A.3.M116PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarDefer, G. L., Widner, H., Marié, R. M., Rémy, P., and Levivier, M. (1999). Core assessment program for surgical interventional therapies in Parkinson’s disease (CAPSIT-PD). Mov. Disord. 14, 572–584. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199907)14:43.0.CO;2-CCrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarDi Fabio, R. P., Kurszewski, W. M., Jorgenson, E. E., and Kunz, R. C. (2004). Footlift asymmetry during obstacle avoidance in high-risk elderly. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 52, 2088–2093. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52569.xPubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarDirnberger, G., and Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: a review. J. Neuropsychol. 7, 193–224. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12028PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarFolstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarFrenkel-Toledo, S., Giladi, N., Peretz, C., Herman, T., Gruendlinger, L., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2005). Treadmill walking as an external pacemaker to improve gait rhythm and stability in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 20, 1109–1114. doi: 10.1002/mds.20507PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarGalna, B., Lord, S., and Rochester, L. (2013a). Is gait variability reliable in older adults and Parkinson’s disease? Towards an optimal testing protocol. Gait Posture 37, 580–585. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.09.025PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarGalna, B., Murphy, A. T., and Morris, M. E. (2013b). Obstacle crossing in Parkinson’s disease: mediolateral sway of the centre of mass during level-ground walking and obstacle crossing. Gait Posture 38, 790–794. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.03.024PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarGalna, B., Peters, A., Murphy, A. T., and Morris, M. E. (2009). Obstacle crossing deficits in older adults: a systematic review. Gait Posture 30, 270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.05.022PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarGauggel, S., Rieger, M., and Feghoff, T.-A. (2004). Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 539–544. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.016469CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarGoetz, C. G., Tilley, B. C., Shaftman, S. R., Stebbins, G. T., Fahn, S., Martinez-Martin, P., et al. (2008). Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov. Disord. 23, 2129–2170. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarHerman, T., Mirelman, A., Giladi, N., Schweiger, A., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2010). Executive control deficits as a prodrome to falls in healthy older adults: a prospective study linking thinking, walking, and falling. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 65A, 1086–1092. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq077PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarHiorth, Y. H., Larsen, J. P., Lode, K., and Pedersen, K. F. (2014). Natural history of falls in a population-based cohort of patients with Parkinson’s disease: an 8-year prospective study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 20, 1059–1064. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.06.023PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarHoehn, M. M., and Yahr, M. D. (1967). Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17, 427–442. doi: 10.1212/WNL.17.5.427CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarHollman, J. H., Watkins, M. K., Imhoff, A. C., Braun, C. E., Akervik, K. A., and Ness, D. K. (2016). A comparison of variability in spatiotemporal gait parameters between treadmill and overground walking conditions. Gait Posture 43, 204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.09.024PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarImms, F. J., Exton-Smith, A. N., and Johnson, A. L. (1977). Falls in the elderly related to postural imbalance. Br. Med. J. 1, 261–264. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.6056.261CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarLim, I., van Wegen, E., de Goede, C., Deutekom, M., Nieuwboer, A., Willems, A., et al. (2005). Effects of external rhythmical cueing on gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Clin. Rehabil. 19, 695–713. doi: 10.1191/0269215505cr906oaPubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarLord, S., Rochester, L., Hetherington, V., Allcock, L. M., and Burn, D. (2010). Executive dysfunction and attention contribute to gait interference in “off” state Parkinson’s Disease. Gait Posture 31, 169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.09.019PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarMaidan, I., Eyal, S., Kurz, I., Geffen, N., Gazit, E., Ravid, L., et al. (2018). Age-associated changes in obstacle negotiation strategies: does size and timing matter? Gait Posture 59, 242–247. doi: 10.1016/J.GAITPOST.2017.10.023PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarMaidan, I., Nieuwhof, F., Bernad-Elazari, H., Reelick, M. F., Bloem, B. R., Giladi, N., et al. (2016). The role of the frontal lobe in complex walking among patients with parkinsons disease and healthy older adults: an fNIRS study. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 30, 963–971. doi: 10.1177/1545968316650426PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarMalatesta, D., Canepa, M., and Menendez Fernandez, A. (2017). The effect of treadmill and overground walking on preferred walking speed and gait kinematics in healthy, physically active older adults. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 117, 1833–1843. doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3672-3PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarMichel, J., Benninger, D., Dietz, V., and van Hedel, H. J. A. (2009). Obstacle stepping in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 256, 457–463. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-0114-0PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarNanhoe-Mahabier, W., Delval, A., Snijders, A. H., Weerdesteyn, V., Overeem, S., and Bloem, B. R. (2012). The possible price of auditory cueing: influence on obstacle avoidance in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 27, 574–578. doi: 10.1002/mds.24935PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarPaker, N., Bugdayci, D., Goksenoglu, G., Demircioǧlu, D. T., Kesiktas, N., and Ince, N. (2015). Gait speed and related factors in Parkinson’s disease. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 27, 3675–3679. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.3675PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarPotocanac, Z., and Duysens, J. (2017). Online adjustments of leg movements in healthy young and old. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 2329–2348. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4967-7PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarPotocanac, Z., Hoogkamer, W., Carpes, F. P., Pijnappels, M., Verschueren, S. M. P., and Duysens, J. (2014). Response inhibition during avoidance of virtual obstacles while walking. Gait Posture 39, 641–644. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.07.125PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarRiley, P. O., Paolini, G., Della Croce, U., Paylo, K. W., and Kerrigan, D. C. (2007). A kinematic and kinetic comparison of overground and treadmill walking in healthy subjects. Gait Posture 26, 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.07.003PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarRochester, L., Nieuwboer, A., Baker, K., Hetherington, V., Willems, A. M., Kwakkel, G., et al. (2008). Walking speed during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease: which characteristics are important? Mov. Disord. 23, 2312–2318. doi: 10.1002/mds.22219PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarSheldon, J. H. (1960). On The Natural History Of Falls In Old Age. Br. Med. J. 2, 1685–1690. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.5214.1685CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarSmulders, K., van Nimwegen, M., Munneke, M., Bloem, B. R., Kessels, R. P. C., and Esselink, R. A. J. (2013). Involvement of specific executive functions in mobility in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 19, 126–128. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.06.010PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarSnijders, A. H., Weerdesteyn, V., Hagen, Y. J., Duysens, J., Giladi, N., and Bloem, B. R. (2010). Obstacle avoidance to elicit freezing of gait during treadmill walking. Mov. Disord. 25, 57–63. doi: 10.1002/mds.22894PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarStegemöller, E. L., Buckley, T. A., Pitsikoulis, C., Barthelemy, E., Roemmich, R., and Hass, C. J. (2012). Postural instability and gait impairment during obstacle crossing in Parkinson’s disease. YAPMR 93, 703–709. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.11.004PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarStolze, H., Klebe, S., Zechlin, C., Baecker, C., Friege, L., and Deuschl, G. (2004). Falls in frequent neurological diseases. J. Neurol. 251, 79–84. doi: 10.1007/s00415-004-0276-8PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarStroop, J. R. (1935). Stroop color word test. J. Exp. Physiol. 18, 643–662.Google Scholarvan Hedel, H., Waldvogel, D., and Dietz, V. (2006). Learning a high-precision locomotor task in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 21, 406–411. doi: 10.1002/mds.20710PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarVitório, R., Pieruccini-Faria, F., Stella, F., Gobbi, S., and Gobbi, L. T. B. (2010). Effects of obstacle height on obstacle crossing in mild Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 31, 143–146. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.09.011PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarWatt, J. R., Franz, J. R., Jackson, K., Dicharry, J., Riley, P. O., and Kerrigan, D. C. (2010). A three-dimensional kinematic and kinetic comparison of overground and treadmill walking in healthy elderly subjects. Clin. Biomech. 25, 444–449. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2009.09.002PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarWeerdesteyn, V., Nienhuis, B., and Duysens, J. (2005a). Advancing age progressively affects obstacle avoidance skills in the elderly. Hum. Mov. Sci. 24, 865–880. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.013PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarWeerdesteyn, V., Nienhuis, B., Mulder, T., and Duysens, J. (2005b). Older women strongly prefer stride lengthening to shortening in avoiding obstacles. Exp. Brain Res. 161, 39–46. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2043-6PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarWood, B. H., Bilclough, J. A., Bowron, A., and Walker, R. W. (2002). Incidence and prediction of falls in Parkinson’s disease: a prospective multidisciplinary study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 72, 721–725. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005521PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarYogev, G., Giladi, N., Peretz, C., Springer, S., Simon, E. S., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2005). Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 1248–1256. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.xPubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google ScholarKeywords: obstacle avoidance, Parkinson’s disease, adaptive gait, treadmill, postural controlCitation: Lu C, Twedell E, Elbasher R, McCabe M, MacKinnon CD and Cooper SE (2019) Avoiding Virtual Obstacles During Treadmill Gait in Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:76. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00076Received: 03 January 2019; Accepted: 19 March 2019;Published: 09 April 2019.Edited by:Eric Yiou, Université Paris-Sud, FranceReviewed by:Jacques Duysens, KU Leuven, BelgiumTeddy Caderby, Université de la Réunion, FranceCopyright © 2019 Lu, Twedell, Elbasher, McCabe, MacKinnon and Cooper. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.*Correspondence: Chiahao Lu, luxxx214@umn.edu†Present address: Emily Twedell, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States Michael McCabe, Trinity School of Medicine, Kingstown, Saint Vincent and the Grenadines
Read Full Article (External Site)
Dr. David Lowemann, M.Sc, Ph.D., is a co-founder of the Institute for the Future of Human Potential, where he leads the charge in pioneering Self-Enhancement Science for the Success of Society. With a keen interest in exploring the untapped potential of the human mind, Dr. Lowemann has dedicated his career to pushing the boundaries of human capabilities and understanding.
Armed with a Master of Science degree and a Ph.D. in his field, Dr. Lowemann has consistently been at the forefront of research and innovation, delving into ways to optimize human performance, cognition, and overall well-being. His work at the Institute revolves around a profound commitment to harnessing cutting-edge science and technology to help individuals lead more fulfilling and intelligent lives.
Dr. Lowemann’s influence extends to the educational platform BetterSmarter.me, where he shares his insights, findings, and personal development strategies with a broader audience. His ongoing mission is shaping the way we perceive and leverage the vast capacities of the human mind, offering invaluable contributions to society’s overall success and collective well-being.